Dooley, David. (2001). Social Research Methods.
(Fourth Edition) Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Pp. xiii + 385.
$79 ISBN 0-13-955428-9
Reviewed by W. Paul Vogt
Illinois State University
July 6, 2001
Dooley's is one of several popular texts introducing
students to research methods in the social sciences. Such
texts inevitably go over much of the same ground. The
interesting differences among them, and the reasons for an
instructor to choose one over another, lie in the emphasis
on, and the depth of coverage of, the various elements of
social research methods.
Two broad categories capture most works published in the
field of research methods texts: 1) books that treat
methodology as a branch of statistics and 2) books that
treat statistics as merely one of several groups of
methods. Dooley's text is an interesting combination. At
first glance it would appear to fit into the category that
subordinates statistics to methodology, because statistics
properly so called is dealt with in an appendix (pp. 315-
340). In addition, chapter 8 on Inferential
Statistics: Drawing Valid Conclusions from Samples
tries to give the reader the bare-bones minimum necessary
set of concepts necessary to read quantitative research
articles and to understand p values. This
minimalist approach will be too casual for some
instructors' tastes, but it has the undoubted advantage of
covering a lot of ground in just a few pages. It will also
popular with students who have formula
anxiety.
On the other hand, despite his treating statistics with a
light hand, the research methods Dooley discusses are all
informed by what we might think of as the statistical model
of reasoning. However informally, he is thinking of
probability distributions and comparing observations to
those distributions. This is his mental model of
inference, and he uses it very consistently and to good
effect. Researchers who are somewhat skeptical of whether
experiments are always the royal road to knowledge might
cavil, but this is Dooley's position, to the point that he
is occasionally guilty of overstating the case: True
experiments generally prove immune to all threats to
internal validity (p. 269, emphasis added). So
Dooley's is a quantitatively oriented book, but one that
does not pursue in much depth the statistical techniques
that undergird most of the methods he discusses.
Qualitative methods, by contrast, are dealt with in one
chapter of 16 pages that mostly discusses ethnography and
participant observation. And even this chapter
investigates the validity of and possible advantages of
qualitative research mostly using the categories of the
statistical model of reasoning. When it is impossible to
apply this statistical model, he essentially says,
qualitative methods can be a fallback that is better than
nothing.
Dooley is strong on the logic of investigation and causal
inference in the social sciences. His theme is
identifying and avoiding rival explanations (p.
49) that threaten the validity of research conclusions,
especially threats to internal validity. In the debate
among teachers of research methods over whether
applications or understanding should be emphasized,
Dooley's book takes less of a hands-on and more
of a brains-on approach. For example, he has a
very sophisticated, though easy to read and brief,
discussion of the problems of drawing inferences from
cross-sectional and longitudinal data (pp. 242-243). His
example discusses the seeming paradox of a direct
correlation between literacy rate and birth rate among
regions in India but an inverse correlation between
literacy and birth rates among individuals living in
those districts. An equally impressive brief account,
given the complexity of the topic, is his discussion of the
comparative value of meta-analysis and traditional
literature reviews (pp. 276-277). The discussion is clear,
balanced, but not really elementary; it goes beyond the
basics without being too difficult for beginning students
to understand.
Another way to categorize research methods texts is
according to the disciplinary background of their authors.
As with many writers of methodology texts, Dooley's
disciplinary base is psychology, but his book contains a
good mix of examples from sociology and economics. His
psychology orientation may, however, have led him to at
least one questionable conclusion. He states, more than
once, that regression toward the mean derives from
measurement error (p. 210). In many circumstances,
this is true; regression toward the mean can be thought of
as a psychometric problem of test-retest measurement error,
especially when the construct is very difficult to measure.
And this may be how regression to the mean is most often
encountered in psychology. But this is not its most
important meaning in other disciplines such as economics
and sociology. Ever since the original studies of height
across generations by Galton, regression to the mean has
indicated partial causation, not error. The reason
Galton's very tall fathers tended to have somewhat shorter
sons was not that people were clumsy when they measured
height in the 19th century. The reason is that fathers' height is
not a perfect determinant of son's height. Unless two
variables are perfectly correlated, there will be
regression toward the mean, as when parents' education
level (measured in standardized units) predicts, but
imperfectly, children's education level. In other words,
regression toward the mean is imperfect correlation.
This is why we symbolize correlation r
(for regression) not c.
On the whole, this is a small point. Such disciplinary
differences in statistical method are not usually
overwhelming. For the most part, statistics is statistics
is statistics. The principles are the same regardless of
what is being studied and in what disciplinefor the
most part. But different disciplines focus on different
aspects of the statistical repertoire.
Dooley's
chapter on program evaluation, which he entitles
applied research, is very solid. His
background in economics and economic psychology shows to
good effect here. As with many other sections of the book,
Dooley's discussion of evaluation research is easy to read,
but not really elementary. He is especially interesting on
the use of evaluation research. Much evaluation
work seems wasted because not used sufficiently by
decision- and policy-makers. If that is the case, then
why continue to conduct evaluations? Dooley
concludes that we need systematic research on how
best to give away evaluation results (pp. 295-
296).
An important positive feature of the book is that each
chapter ends with suggestions of very useful web sites
students can visit, such as one containing student software
for hierarchical linear modeling or another linking to a
web-based sample size estimator. Also quite useful in this
regard is Dooley's Appendix A on Social Research and
the Internet (pp. 301-314).
While being very up-to-date concerning the use of the
Internet, some of the examples of research Dooley uses to
illustrate his points are quite dated. I have no problem
with this, but I find (impressionistic research only) that
my students react quite differently to older studies, even
older research examples nicely exemplify a concept. Given
students' sometimes negative reaction to
historical research, were I writing a new
edition of a textbook, I would try harder to find more
recent illustrationseven when some of my older
examples were very apt. Many of Dooley's examples come
from research published in the 1960s and 1970s, such as his
use of the debate over white flight to
illustrate problems of interpreting data from time series
studies (pp. 217-220). It is a good illustration, but I
find that my students often think there is something odd
about reading a study published before they were born, even
when I try to assure them that it is a
classic.
In sum, Dooley's Social Research Methods covers a
great deal of subject matter in a book that is fairly
short: 340 pages of text including two appendices, 300
pages without appendices (but the appendices are much too
important to skip; they should probably be chapters).
Because of its brevity, the book would make a good text to
use for instructors who assign supplemental readings and
activities. Many of the discussions are quite advanced
while being comparatively easy for students to read.
Dooley accomplishes this by keeping applications and
technical discussions to a minimum and focusing on more
conceptual accounts of research methods.
About the Reviewer
Paul Vogt is Professor of Research Methods and Evaluation
in the Department of Educational Administration and
Foundations at Illinois State University. He is the author
of Dictionary of Statistics and Methodology. Second
Edition, 1999. SAGE Publications.
|
No comments:
Post a Comment