|
Zimmerman, Barry J. & Schunk, Dale H. (Eds).(2003).
Educational Psychology: A Century of Contributions [A
Project of Division 15 (Educational Psychology) of the American
Psychological Association]. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Pp. xiii + 490.
$125 (Cloth) ISBN 0-8058-3681-0
$55.00 (Paper) ISBN 0-8058-3682-9
Reviewed by Naomi Jeffery Petersen
Indiana University, South Bend
November 12, 2004
In the Educator’s Studio: 16 Icons of Educational
Psychology
Before a brief tour of the book’s organization and
content, let’s agree that Educational Psychology: A
Century of Contributions is a gem for its scope, accuracy,
and voice. It is the voices of the authors assembled by editors
Zimmerman and Schunk that make the volume prizeworthy. Many
(including the editors themselves) have also contributed
substantially to the field of educational psychology and thus
warrant a chapter profile in such a collection. Perhaps the
reason the august authors are paying, instead of receiving,
tribute is that they are contributing still; the profiles are of
people nearly all deceased. This brings us to the distinguishing
characteristic of A Century of Contribution its
poignancy.
These are professionally informed yet very personal portraits
in most cases, focusing on the biography as much as the legacy of
each icon. Their goal is not the technicality but the dynamic;
not the products but the process of organizing and beefing up the
perspective of educational psychology: “We want readers to
know these pioneering scholars as real humans who responded to
the challenges of their time in personal and collective ways that
changed the course of educational psychology” (p. vii). The
separate voices range from textbook neutral (Jarvin &
Sternberg on Binet; Gutek on Montessori) to reflective (Pajares
on William James; Bredo on Dewey; Morris on Skinner) to downright
chatty (Berliner on Nate Gage; the editors on Al Bandura).
Whatever the degree of formality, the chapters are unfailingly
respectful if not reverential. If these were broadcast, they
would be In the Actor’s Studio.
Barry Zimmerman and Dale Schunk are no strangers to each other
(e.g. Schunk & Zimerman, 1998; Zimmerman & Schunk, 1989)
nor are they strangers to every education student who ever
Googled ‘self-regulated learning’, their own
significant contribution to the field of educational psychology
(e.g. Schunk, 1991; Zimmerman, 1990). There is every possibility
that Educational Psychology: A Century of Contributions,
which they edited and helped write, will increase their citation
frequency. It’s a keeper and will no doubt be required
reading on many syllabi. The 2001 publication demanded a hefty
$110 in hardcover, but the 2003 softcover edition is half that
price, and therefore in league with most textbooks these days. By
the way, this time it’s Barry’s turn to be first
author.
This is an inspiring if at times opinionated history suitable
for a foundations course in American education, for it
demonstrates the historical paths of the science of psychology
and the philosophies of schooling. More than portraits drawn of
iconic figures, it is a trajectory drawn of brilliant minds
developing powerful models in the context of their circumstances,
their information, and their interactions. This volume
concentrates the legacy. The editors carefully explain in the
preface how the selection was made, based on an advisory board.
Perhaps the board members could be analyzed for their biases, but
the sixteen who made the cut are unquestionably important. They
are to be commended for considering contributions rather than
limiting themselves to the posthumous: unlike others profiled,
Jerome Bruner remains very much with us but is legacy is so great
that he could not be excluded for such a minor deviation as
survival.
If any omissions distract from the power of the collection
(i.e., Lewin, 1935), it is good to respect their focus not people
who profoundly influenced not education but more narrowly the
field of educational psychology, by which they mean generating
further publishable research or the use of rigorous scientific
thinking. The respect for data is a powerful criteria such as
Mayer points out regarding Thorndike:
- The emphasis on reward over punishment had a widespread
influence on educational practice, helping to end the practice of
punishing students for giving incorrect answers. More
importantly, this episode demonstrated the integrity of a
scientist who valued the role of data so much that, when
confronted with overwhelming evidence, he was willing to
acknowledge that his most important discovery was wrong”
(p. 133).
Ultimately, the book is not a series of tributes to exemplary
individuals but a collective cheer for in the relay race against
ignorance. The challenge is issued: What will you contribute in
the next century? What established knowledge will you revise in
the face of new evidence?
Each chronological section begins with an
overview chapter that situates the contributions in theory and
current events to the development of a hybrid field replacing
purely abstract theorizing or concrete folk wisdom. The
divisions of the century into three periods are not marked by
such familiar turning points as technological or political
events, such as Sputnik or Brown v. Topeka. Written by
distinguished authors, these three chapters provide an elegantly
succinct summary that could easily serve as a brief for an
undergraduate course in foundations; the bibliographies alone for
each chapter are also resources in themselves.
- Vernon C. Hall introduces the 1890-1920
‘founding period’ with a succinct four-point context
(Progressive movement, Great War, Technology, and Darwinism)
after which he situates its six biographies –James, Binet,
Dewey, Thorndike, Terman, and Montessori. This first set suggests
the emphases of psychological theory, research methodology, and
pedagogical practice that are the conceptual foundation of the
field.
- William Asher summarizes the 1920-1960
‘rise to prominence’ period with a nod toward the
measurement interest for which he is well known (e.g. 1976; with
Lauer, 1988). The rise refers to the emerging stature of
educational psychology as a focus for publishable works that
influenced the methods and rigor of research, seen in the work
of Skinner, Cronbach, and Gagne. Vygotsky and Piaget are
included in this section; although they were active in their own
countries during this period, their influence in America did not
occur until the next period in which Bruner promoted them in
translation, according to Lutkehaus and Greenfield in Chapter
17.
- Michael Pressley (2001; 2003; Block &
Pressley, 2002; Brainerd & Pressley, 1985; Pressley &
McCormick, 1995) is the voiceover for the most recent era from
1960 to the presentpersonified by Bloom, Gage, Bruner,
Bandura, and Brownwith his characteristic blend of personal
and professional meaningfulness much like his lucid review (1997)
of E.D. Hirsch.
The separate biographical chapters within each
section can then be studied for an in-depth look at each icon of
the field. The paths of discovery, the responses to chance
opportunities, and the human characteristics influencing the work
are detailed as much as the legacy of practical application of
their theories. The decisions they made to pursue a particular
idea are mentioned as if to demonstrate that there are always
more aspects of education to investigate. For instance, the
story of Thorndike’s research on chickens with the generous
mentoring by William James (who housed the chickens in his
basement, thereby providing entertainment for the James’
children in addition to supporting Thorndike’s budding
scholarship) never fails to remind me of the personal and
practical aspects of teaching educational research.
Although some of the details may be found in foundational
texts, typical students may skip over the sidebars of personal
anecdote in their race to find soundbites for an exam. In this
volume, though, the anecdotes are key to understanding the
person, and the person is key to understanding the development of
the field. The portraits are human and the history very real.
One theme is the frequent rejection experienced by creative
thinkers, and the triumphant irony of becoming more highly
regarded than one’s early critics. Yet another feature
common to several portraits is the combination of compassion and
oblivion, of perseverance and impatience, of difficulties
transferring private insight to collective good. Also mentioned
are the accidents and campaigns that lead to individuals becoming
known outside their countries or in the unlikely position to
influence public policy. The entire work demonstrates the fluid
and dynamic function of scholarly discovery and discourse. A
useful companion to A Century of Contribution would be
Flinders and Thorntons’ (1997) Curriculum Studies
Reader, a collection of original texts from these key figures
and more. This would supplement the necessarily restricted
list.
The editors usually matched authors to icons based on existing
scholarly connections, although the connection may not be
immediately obvious. Brainerd is known for his forensic science
(Brainerd & Ornstein, 1991) but his focus is on the
testimony of young children, revealing an enduring interest in
their cognition (1973; Seigel & Brainerd, 1978). In Chapter
11 he extends his study of and centennial tribute to Piaget
(1984; 1996). In contrast, Gerald Gutek (1997; 2004) has written
his own comprehensive texts of educational thought and thus has a
broad general interest; his task of profiling Maria Montessori
remains a comparatively impersonal academic exercise although his
list of references is brief.
Chapters toward the back of the book, i.e.,
featuring the more recent icons, were increasingly memoirs of
shared experience: Zimmerman and Schunk worked closely with
Bandura and they share fond memories. Lorin Anderson (2001) was
part of Bloom’s team developing the famous cognitive
taxonomy, and just recently published a revision after Bloom
succumbed to Alzheimer’s disease; Palincsar was a close
colleague and friend of Anne Brown and her chapter is infused
with grieving remembrance. It is thus a fitting final chapter in
the volume, personifying the personal connection we can all make
to scholars before us.
The author-subject connection in the ‘intellectual
biography of Jerome Bruner’ chapter is noteworthy for its
interdisciplinary aspect. Nancy Lutkehaus is an anthropologist
(e.g. Huber & Lutkehaus, 1999) as is her colleague Patricia
Greenfield . Lutkhaus is current working on curriculum for
Bruner’s MACOS [Man A Course of Study] while Greenfield has
worked with him variously for decades (Bruner, Oliver &
Greenfield, 1966). Of interest is their opening description of
their ‘sources and perspectives” (p. 410) in which
they explain these connections and also what they based their
chapter content on. Greenfield analyzed his four major books;
Lutkehaus is currently working on a book analyzing his impact on
psychological anthropology. Although familiar with The Culture
of Education (1996), we teacher educators might not always
consider the reverse trend of education influencing the fields it
studies.
How does this differ from a standard textbook?
Missing are the laborious exercises, the distracting
illustrations, the self-referential process of insisting it is
somehow linked to achievement. What a relief, and all the more
reason to choose it. An exercise I can imagine posing to teacher
education students would be to organize the information in these
three chapters into timelines and maps, showing interaction
between the people and events, for this is the point of such a
book: To show the dynamic, or even organic, process of developing
a field of study and that all members of the field, if not
humanity, are a part of the endeavor to understand and improve
the way we nurture learning, and that all thoughts are somehow
situated in the context of one’s perspective. An
interesting graphic would be the connections between all of the
authors and subjects, a sort of ‘six degrees of
separation’ exercise. Thus it provides the opportunity for
many creative uses while its structure engages and guides the
reader.
Educational Psychology: A Century of
Contributions, more than any other, reveals the
multiple perspectives of the many who defined the field of
educational psychology. Any educator’s personal library is
richer for including it; library reference shelves cannot be
without it.
References
Anderson, L., Krathwohl, D., Airasian, P., Cruikshank, K.
Mayer, R., Pintrich, P., Raths, J., & Wittrock, M.
(2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing:
A revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy for educational
objectives. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Allyn &
Bacon.
Asher, J. W. (1976).
Educational research and evaluation methods. Boston:
Little, Brown.
Block, C., & Pressley, M. (Eds.). (2002).
Comprehension Instruction: Research-Based Best Practices.
Guilford
Brainerd, C.J. (1973). Neo-Piagetian training experiments
revisited: Is there any support for the cognitive developmental
stage hypothesis? Cognition, 2, 349-70.
Brainerd, C. J. (1978). Learning research and Piagetian
theory. In L.S. Siegel and C.J. Brainerd (Eds.), Alternatives
to Piaget: Critical essays on the theory. New York:
Academic Press.
Brainerd, C. J. (1984). Piaget and instruction. In O.
Selfridge, E. Rissland, & M. Arbib (Eds), Adaptive
systems. New York: Plenum.
Brainerd, C. J. (1996). Piaget: A centennial celebration.
Psychological Science, 7, 191-195.
Brainerd, C.J. & Ornstein, P.A. (1991). Children's memory
for witnessed events: the developmental backdrop. In J. Doris
(Ed.), TheSuggestibility of Children's
Recollections. American Psychological Association:
Washington, D.C. (pp.10-20).
Brainerd, C. J. & Pressley, M. (Eds.). (1985).
Basicprocesses in memory development. New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Bruner, J. (1997). The culture of education
(2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
Bruner, J., Olver, R., & Greenfield, P. (1966). Studies
in cognitive growth. New York: Wiley.
Caldwell, E. C. & Hall, V. C. (1969). The influence of
concept training on letter discrimination.
Child Development, 40. 63-72.
Deutch, M. (1949) ‘A theory of cooperation and
competition’, Human Relations 2: 129-52
Dusek, J. (Ed.), Hall, V. C., & Meyer, W. (1988).
Evolution and learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Greenfield, P. (2004). Weavinggenerations together:
Evolving creativity in the Maya of Chiapas. Santa Fe NM:
School of American Research Press.
Gutek, G . (1997). Philosophical and ideological
perspectives on education. Prentice Hall,
Gutek, G. (2004). Philosophical and ideological voices in
education.London: Pearson.
Hall, V. C. & Tang, S. (1995). The overjustification
effect: A meta-analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9.
365-405.
Hall, V. C., Chiarallo, K. S., & Edmondson, B. (1996)
Deciding where knowledge comes from depends on where you look.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 88. 305-313.
Hall, V.C. (1997). Reflections of an aging
educationalpsychologist.Educational Psychology
Review, 9. 297-311
Huber, M. & Lutkehaus, N. (1999). Gendered missions:
Women and men in missionary discourse and practice.
University of Michigan Press.
Kingsley, R. C. & Hall, V. C. (1967). Training
conservation through the use of learning sets. Child
Development, 38. 1111-27.
Lauer, J. & Asher, J. W. (1988). Composition
research: Empirical designs. Oxford University Press.
Lewin, K. (1935) A dynamic theory of personality. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Pajeres, F. (2001). Toward a positive
psychology of academic motivation. The Journal of Educational
Research, 95.
Pressley, M. & McCormick, C. B. (1995).
Advanced educational psychology for educators, research, and
policymakers. New York: HarperCollins.
Pressley, M. (2001). Reading instruction that works: The
case for balanced teaching (2nd ed.).
Guilford Press.
Pressley, M. (2003) A few
things reading educators should know about instructional
experiments. Reading Teacher, 5. 64-72.
Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation.
Educational Psychologist, 26. 207-232.
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, G. J. (Eds.). (1994).
Self-regulation of learning and performance: Issues and
educational applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Siegel, L.S. and Brainerd, C.J. (Eds.). (1978) Alternatives
to Piaget: Critical essays on the theory. New York:
Academic Press.
Flinders, D., Thornton, S., & Thornton, D. (1997). The
curriculum studies reader. London: Routledge.
Zimmerman, B. J. (1990. Self-regulating academic learning and
achievement: The emergence of a social-cognitive perspective.
Educational Psychology Review, 2: 173-201.
Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (Eds.). (1989).
Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theory,
research and practice. New York: Springer-Verlag.
About the Reviewer
Naomi Jeffery Petersen [NJP@iusb.edu] is
an Assistant Professor of Education at Indiana University South
Bend. Her research interests include classroom educator role
profiles and the roles of adjunct faculty in schools of
education.
| |
No comments:
Post a Comment