Sternberg, Robert J. and Sternberg, Karin. (2008)
The Nature of Hate. NY:
Cambridge University Press
Pp. 246 ISBN 978-0-521-72179-0 Reviewed by Catherine Scott February 6, 2009 In the annals of human civilisation our own era
will probably go down as one of the bloodiest and most vicious.
The sheer number of ‘genocides’ that have been
committed since the turn of the nineteenth century and the
associated body count beggar belief. The most notorious, the
Holocaust, spawned a spate of psychological research into the
causes of that terrible event, in the hope of preventing its
repetition. This included the research by Adorno and his
colleagues into the authoritarian personality; Kohlberg’s
work on moral development; Milgram’s research into
obedience and, most recently, the research by Robert Sternberg
into hate. Sternberg and Sternberg’s book provides a good summary
of the research and theory on hate to date, before presenting and
elaborating on Robert Sternberg’s duplex theory of hate and
summarising the research evidence for it. The book also covers
many of the best-known collective degenerations into hate and
their apparent causes, including some of the most notorious
recent genocides in Rwanda, Sudan and the Balkans. There is a sense in the book, however, that, the demon being
wrestled with has still not been entirely subdued, or at least,
understood. The many and varied examples of people’s
sometimes violent aversion to one or even many of the members own
species are not in the end resolved into a convincing
whole. Wittgenstein’s observation about the sources of
‘philosophical bewilderment’ is of potential use for
understanding the unresolved complexities revealed by Sternberg
and Sternberg’s discussion of hate: ‘We are up
against one of the great sources of philosophical bewilderment: a
substantive makes us look for a thing that corresponds to
it’ (cited in Lazerowitz, 2004, p. 162). The word
‘hate’ does suggest the existence of a unitary
phenomenon, as does our experience of our own strong ill-feeling
towards others, either as individuals or groups. Sternberg and
Sternberg are on to something when they observe that people say
they hate everything from homework to individuals who have
grievously wronged them in some way to abstract entities, like
members of some category of person, but that surely these diverse
experiences can not be identical in content and
intensity. ‘Hate’ like most or all concepts, is
‘fuzzy’, that is, there are no clear cut boundaries
around it nor a set of attributes that are all present in every
case of it: as with all concepts, membership is determined by
‘family resemblances’ (Rosch, 1999). There are, in
other words, a collection of attributes that are characteristic
of people’s experiences of hate but no experience of hate,
whether of homework or members of some despised ethnic group,
necessarily includes all or even most characteristics.
Sternberg’s duplex theory of hate, with its three
components, could be styled as an investigation of the fuzzy
concept of ‘hate’. It is, in other words, a study of
how people think and talk about hate. As such it is of value and
interest. In the end, however, the experience of strong personal
enmity by one individual to another is substantially different to
mass hatred and murder. It may have been desirable to explore
these phenomena separately rather than as manifestations of one
human tendency. The ultimate purpose of the book is search for a way to
‘cure hate’. Because such a wide variety of phenomena
is covered it is unlikely that there exists one panacea that will
cure them all. Certainly therapy may help those individuals who
are afflicted with a propensity to hate or so maimed by
experience that they descend into hate but hate that comes from
collective causes may be much harder to stifle. As an example, the strong temptation for leaders to cook up an
external threat to distract followers from problems on the home
front will not be cured by therapy of any sort: only the
development of a polity sufficiently well-fed and secure, as well
as too sophisticated to fall for such tricks, will eradicate that
source of manufactured hate. Certainly the eradication of
injustice and inequality in all their guises would remove a
powerful incentive to loathing one’s fellows, especially
those who seem to be better off than oneself. The apparently
universal narrative of ‘pure evil’ besetting
‘blameless victims’ that Baumeister so thoroughly
explores (1996) will always be available as a means to explain
why the other is bestial and fit only for extermination.
Collective approaches that address the root causes of division
between peoples may ultimately the best cure for mass hate but we
are still drawn towards explanations and interventions that begin
at the individual level. Indeed, Western psychology as an enterprise is strongly
individualist in its emphasis, not surprising, given that
individualism is the dominant belief system/model of the person
in Western cultures. Psychology starts with the individual as an
explanatory variable and sometimes adds other aspects of the
situation in its attempts to account for human behaviour. Such a
‘bottom up’ model constantly runs into problems,
however, as Sternberg and Sternberg’s analysis
reveals. More convincing explanations for human behaviour become
possible when the bias towards starting with the individual is
put aside and ‘top down’ influences are investigated
as primary causes instead of add-ons, with one, one top down
variable particularly implicated in the bloody nature of our
age. Anthropologist Mary Douglas has explored the key role of
culture in human conduct and has written about the catastrophic
effects of cultural failure, when the usual vibrant conversation
between the four cultural tendencies that she describes in her
grid/group theory falls silent, ordinary life ceases and human
society degenerates into the sort of nightmare Hobbes envisioned
(Douglas, 2004). The key cause over the last century and more of cultural
failure – the disintegration of everyday life - has been
war. Wars have gone from events that affected mostly military
personnel to being an assault upon whole populations. Starting in
the mid-nineteenth century, each subsequent war saw civilian
deaths accounting for higher and higher percentages of the
casualty figures. The ‘evolution’ of weapons from
devices used at close or relatively close quarters and capable of
inflicting injury on one or a few persons to implements able to
destroy a great number of people from a considerable distance
undoubtedly contributed to war’s breaking out of the battle
fields and into the realm of ordinary life, a development
Sternberg and Sternberg comment upon. The disruption or total destruction on of normal existence by
the intrusion of death and horror on a massive scale is
sufficient to explain the degeneration of human behaviour into
viciousness. There is nothing special lurking in our genes or our
natures that explains hatred on a grand scale and the murder that
follows it nearly so well as the reactions of ordinary people to
extraordinary circumstances. When horror becomes the norm it is
not surprising that it is reflected in the conduct of those
afflicted by horrible situations. What would need explaining is a
circumstance where people could see their cities destroyed, their
homes, businesses and farms ruined, their loved ones and
neighbours murdered and mutilated and simply go about their
ordinary business. Sternberg and Sternberg provide evidence for this in their
book when they report on how the war to remove Saddam Hussein
resulted in an upsurge of sectarian hatred between Sunni and
Shiite and of both groups towards the invading forces. Brutality
in the form of suicide bombings and attacks on civilians
increased. Without the experience of a war conducted at close
quarters it highly unlikely that ordinary Iraqis, or indeed
anyone, would voluntarily descend into slaughtering their
fellows. Our age is the age of genocide because our age is the era of
total war. Remove war or at least remove it from the cities and
villages in which people reside, I would contend, and the wave of
mass murder would also decline. This is one powerful method to
cure hate, but one, unfortunately, that lies well beyond the
capacity of any individual, therapist or
otherwise. Sternberg and Sternberg are to be commended for staring horror
in the face and attempting to understand with the laudable
purpose of ridding the world of hate and its
consequences. References Douglas, M. ‘Traditional Culture–Let’s Hear
No More About It’ inRao, V. and M. Walton(Ed.)Culture
and Public Action, Stanford University
Press Heilman, Kenneth M. (2002) Matter of Mind: A Neurologist's
View of Brain-Behavior Relationships Oxford University Press
(OUP)
Lazerowitz, Morris (2004) Philosophy and Illusion London:
Routledge. Rosch, E. (1999). What are concepts? Contemporary
Psychology, 44, 416-417. Dr Catherine Scott, Swinburne Professional Learning, Swinburne University of Technology, PO Box 218, Hawthorn Vic. 3122 Australia |
Friday, August 1, 2025
Sternberg, Robert J. and Sternberg, Karin. (2008) The Nature of Hate. Reviewed by Catherine Scott , Swinburne University of Technology
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
Dowdy-Kilgour, J. (2008). <cite>PhD Stories: Conversations with My Sisters</cite>. Reviewed by Ezella McPherson, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Dowdy-Kilgour, J. (2008). PhD Stories : Conversations with My Sisters . Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press, Inc. Pp. ...
-
Ravitch, Diane. (1996) National Standards in American Education: A Citizen's Guide. Washington: The Brooki...
-
Chomsky, Noam. (2000). Chomsky on MisEducation , (Edited and introduced by Donaldo Macedo). New York: Rowan and...
-
Education Review/Reseñas Educativas/Resenhas Educativas Howe, Kenneth R. (1997) Understanding Equal Educationa...
No comments:
Post a Comment