Saracho, Olivia, & Spodek, Bernard. (Eds.)
(2008).Contemporary perspectives on science and
technology in early childhood education Charlotte, NC:
Information Age Publishing
Pp. xvi + 186 ISBN 9781593116354 Reviewed by Deborah A. Ceglowski November 29, 2008 The purpose of this book is to provide a “critical
analysis of the review of the research on science and
technology” in the field of early childhood education
(Saracho & Spodek, 2008, p. viii). Edited by Olivia Saracho
and Bernard Spodek, the volume, one in the series,
Contemporary Perspectives in Early Childhood, is divided
into two sections: part 1 focuses on science and part 2 on
technology. The book begins with an overview written by the editors that
presents the main issues related to technology and science in
early childhood education. These include: Benchmarks for
science literary (American Association for the Advancement of
Science, 1993); the relationship between science and technology,
technological literacy, and developmentally appropriate
approaches to technology. There are three chapters on science and early childhood education. “Early childhood science process skills: social and developmental considerations” by Ithel Jones, Vickie Lake, and Miranda Lin begins with an overview of Piaget’ theory of child development and its relationship to science education. It is important for teachers to design “learning experiences that help students learn about the nature of scientific inquiries” (Jones et. al, 2008, p. 20). The processing skills that children can acquire and develop include observation, classifying, measuring, communicating, estimating and predicting, and inferring. Young children can also learn the steps in basic scientific inquiry and utilize them during scientific investigations. Peer teaching and positive interactions with teachers are critical components to a quality early childhood science program. “Knowledge acquisition as conceptual change: The case of theory of biology” by Grady Venville is based on the premise that “the concept of life is a fundamental aspect of developing a theory of biology and the process by which this takes place is conceptual change” (p. 42, 2008).
Children begin with limited and
often unrelated bits of biological knowledge and through the
process of conceptual change learning, children develop more
sophisticated and integrated approaches to understanding biology.
The author then poses assertions about conceptual change:
1)four-to five-year old children develop concepts about the
nature of biology which change over time; 2) classroom discussion
among peers and between children and adults helps children to
develop biological theories; 3) children with a developed theory
of biology benefit more from classroom instruction than peers
without a developed theory; 4)children’s linguistic and
cultural backgrounds effect their understanding of biological
principles; 5) children can simultaneously believe in a
scientific and non-scientific explanation regarding a biological
phenomena; and 6) it is easier to influence children’s
biological beliefs than their framework theories. “Affect
and early childhood science education” by Michalinos
Zembylas poses that motivation, interest, and prior experiences
are key elements for children’s developing and sustaining
an interest in science. The chapter explores the historical roots
of the disjuncture of affect and science education and concludes
that new research in affective learning has direct implications
for science education. The four chapters on technology begin with “New technologies in early childhood: partners in play?” by Doris Bergen that focuses on robotic toys and computer software. For infants and toddlers the developmental impact is not well studied; however, the American Pediatric Association issued a recommendation for no screen time for children under two-years-old. During the preschool years, there is not sufficient evidence to suggest that technological materials are more advantageous than traditional preschool materials and at the elementary school level there are several studies that indicate that these two types of technologies may have a positive impact on student learning. By far, the most promising use is for children with disabilities.
In addition to the two methods
investigated in this article, the author notes that many other
forms of technological support are effective in supporting young
children with disabilities. “Engineering and storytellers:
Using robotic manipulative to develop technological fluency in
early childhood” by Marina Bers describes a study of young
children utilizing Lego Mindstorms Robotic Invention Kit
(consisting of a computer program and specialized Lego block with
an imbedded computer chip). Children can design robots on the
computer and then build them with the Lego kit. The author
includes narratives from first and second graders about the
robots they construct and argues that such Lego kits are critical
for young children to develop an understanding robots and
computer technology. Computer assisted instruction is explored in
“Mathematics and technology: supporting learning for
students and teachers” by Douglas Clements and Julie
Sarama. Research supports the effectives of this approach if
children are: 1) provided sufficient time, 2) engage in
multi-modal learning experiences, 3) have software that is
sequential; and 4) receive feedback about their progress.
Computer manipulatives can be used for mathematical instruction
but in any use of mathematical technology, it is essential that
teachers receive ongoing professional development and support.
“Vocabulary learning by computer in kindergarten: The
possibilities of interactive vocabulary books” by Eliane
Segers and Anne Vermeer describes two computer programs developed
for kindergarten children who are Dutch language learners. The
programs consist of short stories comprised of high frequency
words−children answer questions about the content and
correctly sequence pictures that depict the stories. There are
also games that children can select that contain the same high
frequency words. Research indicates that the software packages
assist young Dutch language learners in developing language
fluency. The last chapter in the book, written by the editors, reviews
the research on science and technology and proposes a policy and
research agenda in these areas that includes developing updated
guidelines for educationally and developmentally appropriate
approaches in technology and a national initiative for teaching
science in early childhood programs. Readers interested in research on young children in the areas
of science and technology will find this book helpful. The
authors present a review of pertinent studies. However, some of
the research is based upon older elementary school-aged children
and the application of this work to younger children is
problematic. Several of the authors address current issues in early
childhood education such as young children with disabilities
(Bergen), young language learners (Segers & Vermeer),
technology use for children under two (Bergen; Saracho &
Spodek) and partnerships with families (Bers). The Bergen article
highlights the successful use of simple and complex forms of
technology for young children with disabilities. The Segers and
Vermeer study utilizes several computer programs to teach Dutch
to young children; however, the approach does not offer bilingual
support for young children−arguably the most effective way
to teach young children a second language. Bergen and Saracho and
Spodek mention the American Pediatric Association’s
statement advocating no screen time for children under two years
of age but none of the authors review the research related to
this statement or current research that found some infant/toddler
video programs have a negative impact on certain aspects of
infant development. Bers mentions family involvement at the end
of the article but does not include a discussion about the
research related to home learning. This is particularly salient
given the work of the (Banks group) and their findings that
children’s knowledge is formed primarily from what they
learn in home and community settings, not school. This is
particularly relevant for the multi-lingual, multi-cultural
population of young children enrolled in American early childhood
programs. My favorite chapter, written by Michalinos Zembylas, explores
the relationship between science education and affect. Zembylas
makes a compelling case that dispositions towards learning are as
important as the content itself− in this case, scientific
knowledge. This chapter bridges the gap between the widespread
belief that early childhood education focuses all aspects of
child development through integrated teaching with some of the
recent research that supports the relationship between affect and
learning. About the Reviewer Deborah A. Ceglowski is an Associate Professor of Early
Childhood Education at Ball State University in Muncie, Indiana.
|
Tuesday, July 1, 2025
Saracho, Olivia, & Spodek, Bernard. (Eds.) (2008). Contemporary perspectives on science and technology in early childhood education Reviewed by Deborah A. Ceglowski, Ball State University
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
Strong-Wilson, Teresa. (2008). <cite>Bringing Memory Forward: Storied Remembrance in Social Justice Education with Teachers. </cite> Reviewed by Patricia H. Hinchey, Pennsylvania State University
Strong-Wilson, Teresa. (2008). Bringing Memory Forward: Storied Remembrance in Social Justice Education with Teachers. Ne...
-
Ravitch, Diane. (1996) National Standards in American Education: A Citizen's Guide. Washington: The Brooki...
-
Chomsky, Noam. (2000). Chomsky on MisEducation , (Edited and introduced by Donaldo Macedo). New York: Rowan and...
-
Education Review/Reseñas Educativas/Resenhas Educativas Howe, Kenneth R. (1997) Understanding Equal Educationa...
No comments:
Post a Comment